Andrew Wilson, «Scribal Habits in Greek New Testament Manuscripts.», Vol. 24 (2011) 95-126
New Testament textual criticism lays considerable stress upon the ways that scribes altered the text. Singular readings provide the most objective and reliable guide to the sorts of errors scribes produced. This paper reports on a study of 4200 singular readings from 33 chapters of the New Testament, providing new insights into scribal habits and the history of the text.
112 Andrew Wilson
stylistic effects without changing the sense. However, one-word additions
and omissions with stylistic effects outnumber “stylistic” substitutions
and transpositions. Where omissions (or additions) of larger stretches of
text are involved, the sense is more often affected than the style.
Of the 2279 singular readings investigated, the style was affected in
794 cases. Among these readings, stylistically harsher readings outnum-
bered stylistic more polished readings by 606 to 188 (a ratio of 76:24)50.
We may therefore state that the traditional preference for the harsher
reading is without foundation. Scribes tended to produce a terser and
harsher text, not a smoother one. This is not so much a matter of statis-
tical regularity as it is a matter of logical inevitability. It follows largely
from the fact that scribes tended to omit rather than add to the text.
The majority of these cases consisted of one word and produced only a
harsher text.
Corrections among Harder and Harsher Readings
The results for corrections among the singulars catalogued in relation
to the Harder and Harsher Reading canons were as follows:
Nonsense Harder Sense Harder Style Neutral Easier Style Easier Sense
145 42 100 41 18 0
Correctors removed difficulties – it was their job description. They
heavily tended to deal with nonsense and improve the sense of the text.
Scribes, however, tended to do the exact opposite. They created difficul-
ties; hence the need for correctors.
When corrections amongst singular readings that had a stylistic effect
upon the text were analyzed, there were 100 harsher readings corrected,
as against 18 smoother readings corrected. Correctors themselves created
11 harsher singular readings and 7 smoother singular readings (61:39).
Thus, the countervailing effect of correction was to heavily favour a
smoother text, even more strongly than the original scribe’s effect was to
produce a harsher text, although the correctors themselves also created
additional harsher singular readings. Whether these patterns of correc-
tion are the direct result of correctors making stylistic choices or focusing
their attention on omissions and additions (in and of themselves) is hard
to say.
50
These results were listed by chapter in the table shown earlier under Prefer the Harder
Reading